Aroma Pets

Holistic Therapy For Pets

Articles

Army reveal update on condition of horses that ran amok in London after surgery

Star InactiveStar InactiveStar InactiveStar InactiveStar Inactive

Army reveal update on condition of horses that ran amok in London after surgery

The British Army say the two horses who bolted and ran amok across London are continuing to make good progress.

They confirmed in an update shared today that horses Quaker and Vida were improving. The pair stunned bystanders on April 24 as they ran amok across the city before being recaptured, near Limehouse some five away from where they first bolted.

"Two horses underwent surgery. One, Quaker, a Cavalry black, has shown significant improvement and progresses towards what is expected to be a full recovery," the British Army said in an X post. "The other horse, Vida, a grey, continues to make progress.

"He remains under close and careful professional veterinary observation as his wounds heal. We are so thankful for everyone’s concern and expressions of support, and for all those involved in their care.

"Healing takes time - please be patient as we support that process. The soldiers and horses are all receiving the very best of care."

One of the horses was seen soaked in blood as it ran across London

One of the horses was seen soaked in blood as it ran across London© PA

As reported by the Mirror, the British Army confirmed two of the injured soldiers are still undergoing treatment in hospital although they are expected to make a full recovery. The remainder have since returned to work.

A group of seven horses and six soldiers from the Household Cavalry were undergoing an extended exercise in Belgravia when the animals were spooked by noise coming from a building site. The horses threw four service personnel onto the ground as they ran amok.

Shortly afterward, one of the loose horses ended up smashing into a taxi that was waiting outside the Clermont Hotel, shattering its windscreen. The driver also said a white horse, later identified as Vida, had crashed into a Mercedes people carrier leaving blood splattered down its body.

Jordan Pettitt, 26, said the grey horse was “vividly” stained red with blood and he heard a black horse collide with a taxi. When the horses were recaptured they were taken away for treatment.

Both Quaker and Vida were operated on the night they were recaptured with one being taken back to the barracks and another to an equine hospital. All other horses were closely observed.

Witness Tula, a management consultant from south London, said: "I got off the 172 bus which ends at Aldwych and two horses went flying past. One black, one white. The white one was drenched in blood from the chest down and they were galloping through the traffic at speed.

“People were stopping in the street shocked. The horses were running into fast-moving traffic and seemed terrified. Some unmarked police cars were chasing after them, which didn’t seem to be helping. I felt shocked. It was pretty gruesome. Felt like a weird dream.”

As the British Army shared updates about the horses' condition, a Household Cavalry whistleblower, known by her alias Kate, said the animals were "ready to explode" due to being allegedly kept in poor conditions. "The horses are so nervous," she told LBC. "They are ready to explode. I don't think it's a healthy environment. It doesn't provide what a horse needs, and space."

She also claimed to have seen rats scurrying around about the stalls, loose cables and the horses being given "dirty water," which made them unwell. Kate added the horses would "only have exercise for an hour a day, and very little sunlight." 

Story by Anders Anglesey & Ryan Carroll: Daily Record

Bees understand the benefits of teamwork, study finds

Star InactiveStar InactiveStar InactiveStar InactiveStar Inactive

Bees understand the benefits of teamwork, study finds

Bees have been shown to work together to reach food - worklater1/iStockphoto

Bees have been shown to work together to reach food - worklater1/iStockphoto© Provided by The Telegraph

Scientists found that in both tasks, the partnered bees waited for their bee friend to appear before attempting to reach the reward, suggesting they understand the benefits of teamwork.

Dr Olli Loukola at the University of Oulu said: “We could tell the bees were waiting for their partners’ help, as opposed to just pausing, because control bees that were trained to push the block alone did not pause.

“Comparing the partnered bees group against the control group, we concluded that the paired bumblebees were indeed waiting for their partners.

“Based on our observations, we believe they delay moving the block because they think they cannot do it on their own.

“While it’s possible that they want the other bee to share the load or the reward, our experimental set-up was not designed to investigate this aspect specifically.

“Studying their prosocial behaviour is an interesting avenue for future research.”

Bees understand the benefits of teamwork, a study suggests.

Finnish researchers devised an experiment in which bumblebees were taught to push a small brick out of the way, or navigate a tunnel, to get to a tasty nectar treat.

The bees were taught to either carry out the task alone or with the help of a partner.

Species such as chimpanzees, bottlenose dolphins, Bornean orangutans, brown capuchins, Asian elephants, wolves, and spotted hyaenas have shown evidence of actively coordinated collaboration but it was not known if it was possible in insects.

In the past few years, bees have been found to be far more socially intelligent than previously believed.

In March, researchers at Queen Mary University of London showed that the insects can solve puzzles and then pass on the technique to their hive mates.

Bumblebees were taught to nudge a lever, and then spin a floor-plate to access a reward of sugary water.

They were then able to demonstrate the solution to their fellow bees, so that they too could enjoy the treat, even if they could not work out the puzzle themselves.

In the wild honeybees pass on information about the distance and direction of pollen by using “waggle dancing” but the behaviour was thought to be instinctive.

Experts said the finding proves bumblebees possess levels of cognitive sophistication which was thought to be unique to humans, and shows that cultural transmission may be possible among insect communities.

Bees are known to be capable of carrying out complex learned tasks in a laboratory setting, such as pulling a string or rolling a ball to gain a treat. They have been seen playing with balls, seemingly for fun, without any reward on offer.

The research was published in Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 

Story by Sarah Knapton: The Telegraph: 

What Animals Teach Us About Health and the Science of Healing

Star InactiveStar InactiveStar InactiveStar InactiveStar Inactive

What Animals Teach Us About Health and the Science of Healing

Obesity is a disease of the environment.”— Richard Jackson - Health

Barbara Natterson-Horowitz, a cardiologist at the University of California Los Angeles, believes that her fellow human physicians have much to learn from their veterinary counterparts. These are not separate fields, she argues in her book, coauthored with science writer Kathryn Bowers, Zoobiquity: What Animals Can Teach Us About Health and the Science of Healing.

Did you know that animals get cancer? heart disease? They also faint. Even diseases we think of as uniquely human, like depression, sexual performance, and addiction are found in the animal world. A lot of animals even self-injure when faced with stress or boredom.

When asked, “why should doctors listen to veterinarians,” in a recent interview she responded:

I can speak from my own personal experience. I had spent almost a couple decades being a human doctor, a cardiologist, and I had very little awareness about veterinary medicine. I, like most physicians, only interacted with veterinarians when my own animals got sick….I had this wonderful opportunity to help out at the Los Angeles Zoo, and through that experience I began seeing, both through the patients I was helping with and listening to the veterinarians on their rounds, that they were dealing with heart failure, and cancer, and behavioral disturbances, and infectious diseases, and really essentially the same diseases that I was taking care of in human patients.

Only a century or two ago, many humans and animals were treated by the same practitioner.

However, animal and human medicine began a decisive split around the turn of the twentieth century. Increasing urbanization meant fewer people relied on animals to make a living. Motorized vehicles began pushing work animals out of our daily life. With them went a primary revenue stream for many veterinarians. And in the United States, federal legislation called the Morrill Land-Grant Acts of the late 1800s relegated veterinary schools to rural communities while academic medical centers rapidly rose to prominence in wealthier cities.

Most physicians would never dream of consulting a veterinarian about human diseases.

Most physicians see animals and their illnesses as somehow “different.” We humans have our diseases. Animals have theirs. 

Well that and the undeniable, and unspoken, medical establishments bias against veterinary medicine. Like all humans, doctors can be snobs. The unwritten hierarchy is based on a combination of factors but it’s pretty safe to bet that a veterinarian is below general practitioner.

“We do not like to consider [animals] our equals,” Charles Darwin once remarked. And yet we are animals. In fact, we share most of our genetic makeup with other creatures. Of course, we do learn from animals. Mice are commonly used to better understand human conditions.

Zoobiquity isn’t about animal testing. It’s about the fact that “animals in jungles, oceans, forests, and our homes sometimes get sick—just as we do. Veterinarians see and treat these illnesses among a wide variety of species. And yet physicians largely ignore this. That’s a major blind spot, because we could improve the health of all species by learning how animals live, die, get sick, and heal in their animal settings.”

One example of where we can learn from is why animals get fat and how they get thin.

Fattening in the animal world has enormous potential lessons for humans—including dieters looking to shed a few pounds and doctors grappling with obesity, one of the most serious and devastating health challenges of our time.

Millions cope with this life-threatening epidemic. Millions of domestic animals that is. These pets are “fatter than ever before, and steadily gaining more weight.” While hard to determine, studies put the number of overweight and obese dogs and cats somewhere between 25 and 40 percent. In case you’re wondering, that’s still, at least for now, well below the proportion of U.S. human adults who are now either overweight or obese, which is closer to 70 percent.

What sets domestic animals apart from their wild cousins? We feed them.

They are mostly or completely dependent on humans for every meal, and we regulate both the quality and the quantity of everything that passes their lips and beaks. Consequently, we can’t really blame them for their weight problems. … And so we’re left with one conclusion: we, the species that both manipulates food to make it more unhealthful and has the intelligence to understand that we shouldn’t eat so much of it, are to blame. We’re responsible not only for our own expanding waistlines but for those of our animal charges as well.

It’s easy and pleasing to assume that animals in their native environments effortlessly stay lean and healthy. That’s not the case.

Abundance plus access—the twin downfalls of many a human dieter—can challenge wild animals, too.

Although we may think of food in the wild as hard to come by, at certain times of the year and under certain conditions, the supply may be unlimited.

So wild animals get fat the same way we humans do: access to abundant food.

Of course, animals also fatten normally—and healthily—in response to seasonal and life cycles. But what’s key is that an animal’s weight can fluctuate depending on the landscape around it.

Learning from animals, call it the zoobiquitous approach, we learn that “weight is not just a static number on a chart. Rather, it’s a dynamic, ever-changing reaction to a huge variety of external and internal processes ranging from the cosmic to the microscopic.”

Richard Jackson says “Obesity is a disease of the environment.” In 2010 he explained what he meant:

One of the problems with the obesity epidemic is we too often blame the victim. And yes, every one of us ought to have more self-control and ought to exert more willpower. But when everyone begins to develop the same set of symptoms, it’s not something in their mind, it’s something in our environment that is changing our health. And what’s changing in our environment is that we have made dangerous food, sugar-laden food, high-fat food, high-salt food … and we’ve made it absolutely the easiest thing to buy, the cheapest thing to buy, and yes, it tastes good, but it’s not what we should be eating.

In a 2009 book, The End of Overeating, David Kessler made a similar point: excess sugar, fat, and salt “hijack our brains and bodies and drive cycles of appetite and desire that make it nearly impossible to resist certain fattening foods.” In a new book I’ve just started reading, Salt Sugar Fat: How the Food Giants Hooked Us, Michael Moss makes the same point. (In case you’re wondering, the calories in calories out argument is bunk.)

One of the lessons we can take away

If you want to lose weight the wild animal way, decrease the abundance of food around yourself and interrupt your access to it. And expend lots of energy in the daily hunt for food. In other words: change your environment.

Nassim Taleb makes a similar point in his book Anti-Fragile:

Perhaps what we mostly need to remove is a few meals at random, or at least avoid steadiness in food consumption. The error of missing nonlinearities is found in two places, in the mixture and the frequency of food intake.

 

The problem with the mixture is as follows. We humans are said to be omnivorous, compared to more specialized mammals, such as cows and elephants and lions. But such ability to be omnivorous had to come in response to more variegated environments with unplanned, haphazard, and, what is key, serial availability of sources—specialization is the response to a very stable habitat free of abrupt changes, redundancy of pathways the response to a more variegated one. Diversification of function had to come in response to variety. And a variety of certain structure.

 

Note a subtly in the way we are built: the cow and the other herbivores are subjected to much less randomness than the lion in their food intake; they eat steadily but need to work extremely hard in order to metabolize all these nutrients, spending several hours a day just eating. … The lion, on the other hand, needs to rely on more luck; it succeeds in a small percentage of the kills, less than 20 percent, but when it eats, it gets in a quick and easy way all these nutrients produced thanks to very hard and boring work by the prey. So take the following principles derived from the random structure of the environment: when we are herbivores, we eat steadily; but when we are predators we eat more randomly. Hence our proteins need to be consumed randomly for statistical reasons.

 

So if you agree that we need “balanced” nutrition of a certain combination, it is wrong to immediately assume that we need such balance at every meal rather than serially so. … There is a big difference between getting them together at every meal … or having them separately, serially.

 

Why? Because deprivation is a stressor—and we know what stressors do when allowed adequate recovery. Convexity effects at work here again: getting three times the daily dose of protein in one day and nothing the next two is certainly not biologically equivalent to “steady” moderate consumption if our metabolic reactions are nonlinear.

… I am convinced that we are antifragile to randomness in food delivery and composition—at least over a certain range or number of days.

We’ve all known that antibiotics are used to stop the spread of certain diseases. But, Zoobiquity, offers another explanation:

Antibiotics don’t kill just the bugs that make animals sick. They also decimate beneficial gut flora. And these drugs are routinely administrated even when infection is not a concern. The reason may surprise you. Simply by giving antibiotics, farmers can fatten their animals using less feed. The scientific jury is still out on exactly why these antibiotics promote fattening, but a plausible hypothesis is that by changing the animals’ gut microflora, antibiotics create an intestine dominated by colonies of microbes that are calorie-extraction experts. This may be why antibiotics act to fatten not just cattle, with their multistomached digestive systems, but also pigs and chicken, whose GI tracts are more similar to ours.

 

This is really a key point: antibiotic use can change the weight of farm animals. It’s possible that something similar occurs in other animals—namely, us. Anything that alters gut flora, including but not limited to antibiotics, has implications not only for body weight but for other elements of our metabolism, such as glucose intolerance, insulin resistance and abnormal cholesterol.

The diet and exercise dogma:Health

Even without an assist from 32-ounce sodas, the yellow-bellied marmots in the Rockies, blue whales off the coast of California and country rats in Maryland have gotten steadily chubbier in recent years. The explanation might lie in the disruption of circadian rhythms. Of the global dynamics controlling our biological clocks — including temperature, eating, sleeping and even socializing — no “zeitgeber” is more influential than light.

The cycle

Modern, affluent humans have created a continuous eating cycle, a kind of “uniseason.” … Sugar is abundant, whether in our processed foods or in beautiful whole fruits that have had their inconvenient seeds bread out of them and that “unzip” from easy-to-peel skins and pop open into ready to eat segments. Protein and fat are everywhere available—in eternal harvest the prey never grows up and learns to run away or fight us off. Our food is stripped of microbes, and we remove more while scrubbing off dirt and pesticides. Because we control it, the temperature is always a perfect 74 degrees. Because we’re in charge, we can safely dine at tables aglow in light long after the sun goes down. All year round, our days are lovely and long; our nights are short.

 

As animals, we find this single season an extremely comfortable place to be. But unless we want to remain in a state of continual fattening, with accompanying metabolic diseases, we will have to pry ourselves out of this delicious ease.

The Evolution of Flight Behavior in Butterflies

Star InactiveStar InactiveStar InactiveStar InactiveStar Inactive

  

The Evolution of Flight Behavior in Butterflies

According to research, non-edible butterfly species that imitate one another's color patterns have developed comparable flight movements as a means of alerting predators and evading extinction.

 team looked specifically at a tribe of butterflies called the Heliconiini. Image Credit: University of York© Provided by AZoLifeSciences

It is commonly recognized that a large number of non-edible butterfly species have developed nearly identifiab{youtube}le color patterns that serve as alerts to potential predators, preventing the butterflies from being consumed.

It has now been demonstrated by researchers at the University of York that these butterflies have developed identical flight patterns in addition to similar color patterns, which combined provide a more potent warning to potential predators.

Researchers at the University of York measured the wing beat frequency and wing angles of 351 butterflies, representing 38 species that fall into one of ten different color pattern mimicry groups. They did this by using high-speed video footage to capture the flight of natural butterflies in South America.

Using this dataset, scientists looked into the relationships between habitat, wing shape, temperature, and the color pattern of the butterfly's “mimicry group” to determine which characteristics had the greatest impact on the butterfly's flight behavior.

The researchers discovered that contrary to expectations, the color pattern mimicry group a butterfly belonged to was the primary determinant of its flight behavior, even if the species’ habitat and wing shape also played a significant role.

This indicates that compared to closely related species that exhibit distinct warning coloring, distantly related butterflies that belong to the same color pattern mimicry group have more similar flight behavior. The butterflies would appear identcal to a predator due to their color patterns, and they would likewise move in a similar way.

The team looked specifically at a tribe of butterflies called the Heliconiini. Image Credit: University of York

Nasty Taste

From an evolutionary perspective it makes sense to share the color pattern between species, to reduce the individual cost of educating predators to the fact that they do not taste nice! Once a predator has tasted one, the visual clues on others indicate that they too are also inedible, but flight patterns are more complex and are influenced by several other factors such as the air temperature and the habitat the species fly in.”

Edward Page, PhD Student and Study Lead Co-Author, Department of Biology, University of York

Edward Page said, “We wanted to see whether flight corresponded to color - could predators be driving the mimicry of flight as well as color patterns? We were surprised to find just how strong and widespread the behavioral mimicry is.”

70 Million Years Ago

The researchers focused on the Heliconiini, a tribe of butterflies found in the Neotropics that comprise over 100 species and subspecies, all of which belong to different groups that replicate different color patterns.

Additionally, they looked into a few species of ithomiine butterflies, which broke away from the Heliconiini some 70 million years ago but share striking similarities in their "Tiger" color patterns.

Sharing flight behavior across multiple species seems to reinforce this ‘do not eat me’ message. It is fascinating that this behavior has evolved between distant relatives over a long period of time, but we can also see flight behavior diverging between differently patterned populations within a species over a relatively short period of time too."

Edward Page, PhD Student and Study Lead Co-Author, Department of Biology, University of York

Subtle Changes

The extent of flight mimicry in this group of butterflies is amazing. It is a great example of how evolution shapes behavior, with selection from predators driving subtle changes which enhance the survival of individuals and the challenge and interest now is to identify the genes causing these changes, which will tell us how such behavioral mimicry evolves.”

Reference: Azo Life Sciences: Kanchon Dasmahapatra, Professor, Department of Biology, University of York

Nightmare fish may explain how our 'fight or flight' response evolved

Star InactiveStar InactiveStar InactiveStar InactiveStar Inactive

Nightmare fish may explain how our 'fight or flight' response evolved

Contrary to popular belief, lampreys, a type of ancient, jawless fish, may have a "flight or fight" response similar to people.

Contrary to popular belief, lampreys, a type of ancient, jawless fish, may have a "flight or fight" response similar to people.© Yiming Chen via Getty Images

Lampreys are the stuff of nightmares, complete with long, slimy bodies; circular mouths filled with teeth; and parasitic tendencies. But lampreys are also vertebrates, which means they have backbones and share a common ancestor with humans — and new research is revealing that we have more in common with these slippery bloodsuckers than scientists previously thought.

Lampreys belong to an ancient vertebrate lineage known as Agnatha, or jawless fish. Previous research suggests that lampreys and their relatives represent the most primitive group of vertebrates still in existence, having evolved an estimated 360 million years ago. These living fossils can give us a window into how some of our distant ancestors likely evolved.

For the last 150 years, scientists assumed that lampreys lacked a jaw because they were missing a structure known as the neural crest. This group of stem cells is unique to vertebrates, and in the womb or the egg, it develops into a wide array of structures. These structures include both jaws and the sympathetic nervous system, which controls our involuntary fight-or-flight response that kicks on in dangerous or stressful situations.

But a new study, published Wednesday (April 17) in the journal Nature, reveals that lampreys have sympathetic nerve cells after all — suggesting that the vertebrate flight-or-flight response is more ancient than scientists expected.

"Studies like this help teach us how we were built over evolutionary time," Jeramiah Smith, a computational biologist at the University of Kentucky who was not involved in the research, told Live Science.

The new study did not begin as a search for sympathetic nerve cells.

"One of the things I love about science is that you often make discoveries by accident," Marianne Bronner, a developmental biologist at Caltech and co-author of the study, told Live Science. Instead, the work started as a search for similar cells that were precursors to the more complex neural crest seen in jawed vertebrates. They thought they might find such cells in lampreys because they are the closest thing we have to ancient jawless vertebrates that first emerged around 500 million years ago.

But when the researchers started dissecting lamprey larvae, they noticed the immature fish had structures that looked a lot like neurons running in a chain down the length of their bodies. This string of nerve cells is characteristic of a sympathetic nervous system — a system lampreys weren't supposed to have.

When the scientists looked closer, they confirmed that these structures were indeed nerves using RNA sequencing; RNA is a cousin of DNA that helps cells make proteins, in addition to serving other functions. The team also found that the cells make a precursor enzyme for noradrenaline, a key chemical messenger that helps control the fight-or-flight response.

"Now it looks like the only thing that lampreys don't have is a jaw," Bronner said.

Lampreys were previously assumed to react to danger by relying solely on pheromones given off by other lampreys. (Ecologists still sometimes use these pheromones to control the critters' movements in the lab.) The discovery that these jawless fish have a fight-or-flight response places the evolutionary origin of this system about 50 million years earlier than scientists expected.

Bronner thinks that past researchers probably missed the sympathetic nerve cells in lampreys for a couple reasons. One is that the fish have a long developmental cycle; after a young lamprey hatches, it can spend years developing in a larval stage before maturing into an adult. The sympathetic neurons may be too small to notice until late in this developmental phase, and most prior research was done on newly hatched lampreys. The new work uncovered the cells in older larvae.

Another issue is that jawless fish are far less studied in evolutionary biology than "model organisms" like fruit flies and zebrafish, which serve as a model for biological systems also found in humans. Such species are great for lab work, especially as scientists know their genomes so well. But Bronner sees huge scientific benefits in studying creatures like lampreys, too.

"Sometimes you have to go outside of your comfort zone and work on these weird animals," Bronner said — nightmare fuel and all. So the next time your adrenaline spikes when you're watching a horror movie or you've heard a twig snap in the woods, consider thanking a lamprey.

Story by Joanna Thompson: Live Science:  

Login

Login

Who's On Line

We have 23 guests and no members online

Featured Video

 


Canine Feeding Schedules

Essential Oils

Zone - Text

We use cookies on our website. Some of them are essential for the operation of the site, while others help us to improve this site and the user experience (tracking cookies). You can decide for yourself whether you want to allow cookies or not. Please note that if you reject them, you may not be able to use all the functionalities of the site.

Ok
X

Right Click

No right click